Friday, November 22, 2019
Why your brain never runs out of problems to find
Why yur brain never runs out of problems to findWhy your brain never runs out of problems to findWhy do many problems in life seem to stubbornly stick around, no matter how hard people work to fix them? It turns out that a quirk in the way human brains process information means that when something becomes rare, we sometimes see it in mora places than ever.Think of a neighborhood watch made up of volunteers who call the police when they see anything suspicious. Imagine a new volunteer who joins the watch to help lower crime in the area. When they first start volunteering, they raise the alarm when they see signs of serious crimes, like assault or burglary.Lets assume these efforts help and, over time, assaults and burglaries become rarer in the neighborhood. What would the volunteer do next? One possibility is that they would relax and stop calling the police. After all, the serious crimes they used to worry about are a thing of the past.But you may share the intuition my research gro up had that many volunteers in this situation wouldnt relax just because crime went down. Instead, theyd start calling things suspicious that they would never have cared about back when crime was high, like jaywalking or loitering at night.You can probably think of many similar situations in which problems never seem to go away, because people keep changing how they define them. This is sometimes called concept creep, or moving the goalposts, and it can be a frustrating experience. How can you know if youre making progress solving a problem, when you keep redefining what it means to solve it? My colleagues and I wanted to understand when this kind of behavior happens, why, and if it can be prevented.After violent crime starts going down, loiterers and jaywalkers may start to seem more threatening.Marc Bruxelle/Shutterstock.comLooking for troubleTo study how concepts change when they become less common, we brought volunteers into our laboratory and gave them a simple task to look a t a series of computer-generated faces and decide which ones seem threatening. The faces had been carefully designed by researchers to range from very intimidating to very harmless.As we showed people fewer and fewer threatening faces over time, we found that they expanded their definition of threatening to include a wider range of faces. In other words, when they ran out of threatening faces to find, they started calling faces threatening that they used to call harmless. Rather than being a consistent category, what people considered threats depended on how many threats they had seen lately.This kind of inconsistency isnt limited to judgments about threat. In another experiment, we asked people to make an even simpler decision whether colored dots on a screen were blue or purple.As the context changes, so do the boundaries of your categories.David Levari, CC BY-NDAs blue dots became rare, people started calling slightly purple dots blue. They even did this when we told them blue do ts were going to become rare, or offered them cash prizes to stay consistent over time. These results suggest that this behavior isnt entirely under conscious control otherwise, people would have been able to be consistent to earn a cash prize.Expanding what counts as immoralAfter looking at the results of our experiments on facial threat and color judgments, our research group wondered if maybe this was just a funny property of the visual system. Would this kind of concept change also happen with non-visual judgments?To test this, we ran a final experiment in which we asked volunteers to read about different scientific studies, and decide which were ethical and which were unethical. We were skeptical that we would find the same inconsistencies in these kind of judgments that we did with colors and threat.Why? Because moral judgments, we suspected, would be more consistent across time than other kinds of judgments. After all, if you think violence is wrong today, you should still t hink it is wrong tomorrow, regardless of how much or how little violence you see that day.But surprisingly, we found the same pattern. As we showed people fewer and fewer unethical studies over time, they started calling a wider range of studies unethical. In other words, just because they were reading about fewer unethical studies, they became harsher judges of what counted as ethical.The brain likes to make comparisonsWhy cant people help but expand what they call threatening when threats become rare? Research from cognitive psychology and neuroscience suggests that this kind of behavior is a consequence of the basic way that our brains process information we are constantly comparing what is front of us to its recent context.Instead of carefully deciding how threatening a face is compared to all other faces, the brain can just store how threatening it is compared to other faces it has seen recently, or compare it to some average of recently seen faces, or the most and least threa tening faces it has seen. This kind of comparison could lead directly to the pattern my research group saw in our experiments, because when threatening faces are rare, new faces would be judged relative to mostly harmless faces. In a sea of mild faces, even slightly threatening faces might seem scary.It turns out that for your brain, relative comparisons often use less energy than absolute measurements. To get a sense for why this is, just think about how its easier to remember which of your cousins is the tallest than exactly how tall each cousin is. Human brains have likely evolved to use relative comparisons in many situations, because these comparisons often provide enough information to safely navigate our environments and make decisions, all while expending as little effort as possible.Being consistent when it countsSometimes, relative judgments work just fine. If you are looking for a fancy restaurant, what you count as fancy in Paris, Texas, should be different than in Paris , France.What once seemed banal can be recategorized as a threat in a new context.louis amal on Unsplash, CC BYBut a neighborhood watcher who makes relative judgments will keep expanding their concept of crime to include milder and milder transgressions, long after serious crimes have become rare. As a result, they may never fully appreciate their success in helping to reduce the problem they are worried about. From medical diagnoses to financial investments, modern humans have to make many complicated judgments where being consistent matters.How can people make more consistent decisions when necessary? My research group is currently doing follow-up research in the lab to develop more effective interventions to help counter the strange consequences of relative judgment.One potential strategy When youre making decisions where consistency is important, define your categories as clearly as you can. So if you do join a neighborhood watch, think about writing down a list of what kinds of transgressions to worry about when you start. Otherwise, before you know it, you may find yourself calling the cops on dogs being walked without leashes.David Levari, Postdoctoral Researcher in Psychology, Harvard UniversityThis article is republished from The Conversation under a Creative Commons license. Read the original article.
Subscribe to:
Post Comments (Atom)
No comments:
Post a Comment
Note: Only a member of this blog may post a comment.